糖心Vlog

Boo! How Do Mexican Cavefish Escape Predators?

Mexican Cavefish, Caves, Escape Behavior, Startle Reflex, Cavefish Populations, Surface Fish

A. mexicanus exist as surface fish (bottom) that inhabit rivers in Mexico and Southern Texas and as more than 30 geographically isolated cave鈥恉welling (top) populations of the same species.


By gisele galoustian | 10/29/2020

The ability to detect threatening stimuli and initiate an escape response is critical for survival and under stringent evolutionary pressure. To detect predators, fish use a number of sensory systems including olfaction (smell) and vision, which contribute to the activation of arousal systems. Surprisingly, little is known about the neural mechanisms through which ecological perturbation shapes the evolution of escape response. When startled, do all fish respond the same way?聽

A few fish, like Mexican cavefish, Astyanax mexicanus, have evolved in unique environments without any predators. To determine how this lack of predation impacts escape responses that are highly stereotyped across fish species, researchers from 糖心Vlog鈥檚 and Harriet L. Wilkes Honors College explored the tiny A. mexicanus聽to determine if there are evolved differences in the species. A. mexicanus聽exist as surface fish that inhabit rivers in Mexico and Southern Texas and as more than 30 geographically isolated cave鈥恉welling populations of the same species.

The ecology of caves differs dramatically from the surface habitat, resulting in distinct morphological and behavioral phenotypes in A. mexicanus. Cave populations live in environments without light, which is thought to contribute to the evolution of albinism, eye鈥恖oss, and circadian rhythm. Because they lack predators they might also lack the selective pressure to avoid predators. Dramatic differences in these cavefish populations combined with the robust ecological differences suggest that the startle reflex could indeed differ between populations of A. mexicanus.

To put this theory to the test, researchers elicited 鈥淐鈥恠tart鈥 responses using acoustic stimuli and high-speed videography in multiple cavefish populations and compared responses to eyed surface fish. The C鈥恠tart escape response represents a primary mechanism for predator avoidance in fish and amphibians. C-start gets its name from the 鈥渃鈥恠haped鈥 curve a fish鈥檚 body forms during the first stage of the escape response, which is followed by a smaller counter鈥恇end and then rapid swimming.

Results of the study, published in a special issue of the on the evolution of Mexican cavefish, support the idea that ecological differences between cave and river environments contribute to differences in A. mexicanus escape behaviors. Findings provide a platform for investigating the evolution of neural circuits contributing to sensory鈥恗otor integration and support using A. mexicanus as a model to investigate the evolution of escape behavior.聽 聽

In diverse fish species, acoustic stimuli activate Mauthner neurons, which initiate a C鈥恠tart escape response. Mauthner cells receive input from multiple sensory modalities including visual, olfactory, and mechanosensory systems. To compare C鈥恠tart kinematics between surface fish and cavefish, researchers examined and quantified response latency, maximum change in orientation (referred to as 鈥減eak bend angle鈥) and angular speed. They used six-day post-fertilization larvae instead of adult surface fish and cavefish to eliminate the confounding variable of learned behavior.

Researchers found that C-start is present in river-dwelling surface fish and multiple populations of cavefish, but response kinematics and probability differed between populations. The Pach贸n population of cavefish exhibited an increased response probability, a slower response latency and speed, and reduction of the maximum bend angle, revealing evolved differences between surface and cave populations. Analysis of the responses of two other independently evolved populations of cavefish, revealed the repeated evolution of reduced angular speed. Investigation of surface鈥恈ave hybrids showed a correlation between angular speed and peak angle, suggesting that these two kinematic characteristics are related at the genetic or functional levels.

鈥淭hese findings show that even the most highly conserved behaviors have evolved differences in the cave environment,鈥 said , Ph.D., lead author and an associate professor of biological sciences, 糖心Vlog鈥檚 Charles E. Schmidt College of Science. 鈥淚t puts us in a position to identify the biological causes that underlie the reduced response probability and other changes we observed between cavefish and surface fish.鈥

Keene notes that perhaps most interesting, is the difference in response probability in which Molino cavefish which diverged from surface fish more recently, were significantly more responsive than any of the other populations, with Pach贸n larvae, originating from the more ancestral stock, exhibiting the second highest response probability.

鈥淭he finding that multiple populations independently evolved similar phenotypes gives us a way to examine how escape circuits can be modified,鈥 said Keene.

Despite having no eyes, cavefish could detect light and sense looming stimuli, raising the possibility that light modulates their C鈥恠tart response. To assess the influence of visual input on the C鈥恠tart responses of surface fish and cavefish, researchers assayed both populations under light and dark conditions. The presence of light had no detectable effect on response probability, response latency, or angular speed in cavefish or surface fish. It did however, influence peak bend angle. In the dark conditions, surface fish displayed an increase in peak bend angle compared to cavefish. Although they were able to perceive light at this age, the presence or absence of light had no observable effect on the responses of Pach贸n cavefish.

To assess differences in responsiveness to acoustic stimuli, researchers quantified the probability of C鈥恠tart initiation in surface fish and Pach贸n cavefish at multiple vibration intensities. They found that Pach贸n cavefish were more likely than surface fish to initiate a C鈥恠tart in response to vibrations of higher intensities (31 and 35 decibels, but not 28 decibels).

鈥淚nterestingly, all cave populations analyzed in our study exhibited decreased angular speed. Furthermore, it is likely that a slower latency, such as that of Pach峤筺 larvae, decreases the likelihood of successful evasion,鈥 said Alexandra Paz, first author, an 糖心Vlog doctoral student and a research assistant in the Department of Biological Sciences. 鈥淭hese data suggest that the C鈥恠tart responses of cavefish, especially from the Pach峤筺 population, may be less effective for successful predator evasion. Because no predators have been identified in the caves, it is possible that this change is a result of diminished predation in the cave environment.鈥

Co-authors of the study are Brittnee McDole, Ph.D., a post-doctoral fellow at 糖心Vlog; , Ph.D., an assistant professor of biology; and Erik R. Duboue, Ph.D., an assistant professor of biology, both in 糖心Vlog鈥檚 Harriet L. Wilkes Honors College.聽

This research was funded by the National Institutes of Health (1R21NS105071 and 1R01GM127872).

To assess differences in responsiveness to acoustic stimuli, researchers quantified the probability of C鈥恠tart initiation in surface fish and Pach贸n cavefish at multiple vibration intensities. They produced acoustic stimuli that then provoked startle responses. Because the main purpose of this response is to avoid predators it happens extremely quickly. Researchers recorded 1,000 frames per second 鈥 the videos are slowed down to 1/30 of the speed. The red light allows researchers to know when exactly the acoustic stimulus is occurring. They strategically placed and covered it so that it was visible to the camera, but not the fish. This allowed the researchers to make precise measurements of exactly how much time passes between the stimulus starting and the fish responding. (Credit: Alexandra Paz)

-糖心Vlog-